The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence

S. Gray, S. Green, Mary Alt, T. Hogan, T. Kuo, S. Brinkley, N. Cowan

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

37 Citations (Scopus)

Abstract

This study investigated the structure of working memory in young school-age children by testing the fit of three competing theoretical models using a wide variety of tasks. The best fitting models were then used to assess the relationship between working memory and nonverbal measures of fluid reasoning (Gf) and visual processing (Gv) intelligence. One hundred sixty-eight English-speaking 7–9 year olds with typical development, from three states, participated. Results showed that Cowan's three-factor embedded processes model fit the data slightly better than Baddeley and Hitch's (1974) three-factor model (specified according to Baddeley, 1986) and decisively better than Baddeley's (2000) four-factor model that included an episodic buffer. The focus of attention factor in Cowan's model was a significant predictor of Gf and Gv. The results suggest that the focus of attention, rather than storage, drives the relationship between working memory, Gf, and Gv in young school-age children. Our results do not rule out the Baddeley and Hitch model, but they place constraints on both it and Cowan's model. A common attentional component is needed for feature binding, running digit span, and visual short-term memory tasks; phonological storage is separate, as is a component of central executive processing involved in task manipulation. The results contribute to a zeitgeist in which working memory models are coming together on common ground (cf. Cowan, Saults, & Blume, 2014; Hu, Allen, Baddeley, & Hitch, 2016).

Original languageEnglish (US)
Pages (from-to)183-201
Number of pages19
JournalJournal of Memory and Language
Volume92
DOIs
StatePublished - Feb 1 2017

Fingerprint

Intelligence
Short-Term Memory
intelligence
Data storage equipment
Buffers
Theoretical Models
Zeitgeist
Young children
Working Memory
Processing
school
manipulation
speaking
Focus of Attention
School-age children
Fluids
Testing

Keywords

  • Children
  • Episodic buffer
  • Nonverbal intelligence
  • Phonological loop
  • Visuospatial sketchpad
  • Working memory

ASJC Scopus subject areas

  • Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology
  • Language and Linguistics
  • Experimental and Cognitive Psychology
  • Linguistics and Language
  • Artificial Intelligence

Cite this

The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence. / Gray, S.; Green, S.; Alt, Mary; Hogan, T.; Kuo, T.; Brinkley, S.; Cowan, N.

In: Journal of Memory and Language, Vol. 92, 01.02.2017, p. 183-201.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Gray, S. ; Green, S. ; Alt, Mary ; Hogan, T. ; Kuo, T. ; Brinkley, S. ; Cowan, N. / The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence. In: Journal of Memory and Language. 2017 ; Vol. 92. pp. 183-201.
@article{139e9e832e53401db9e9cc82270f54c2,
title = "The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence",
abstract = "This study investigated the structure of working memory in young school-age children by testing the fit of three competing theoretical models using a wide variety of tasks. The best fitting models were then used to assess the relationship between working memory and nonverbal measures of fluid reasoning (Gf) and visual processing (Gv) intelligence. One hundred sixty-eight English-speaking 7–9 year olds with typical development, from three states, participated. Results showed that Cowan's three-factor embedded processes model fit the data slightly better than Baddeley and Hitch's (1974) three-factor model (specified according to Baddeley, 1986) and decisively better than Baddeley's (2000) four-factor model that included an episodic buffer. The focus of attention factor in Cowan's model was a significant predictor of Gf and Gv. The results suggest that the focus of attention, rather than storage, drives the relationship between working memory, Gf, and Gv in young school-age children. Our results do not rule out the Baddeley and Hitch model, but they place constraints on both it and Cowan's model. A common attentional component is needed for feature binding, running digit span, and visual short-term memory tasks; phonological storage is separate, as is a component of central executive processing involved in task manipulation. The results contribute to a zeitgeist in which working memory models are coming together on common ground (cf. Cowan, Saults, & Blume, 2014; Hu, Allen, Baddeley, & Hitch, 2016).",
keywords = "Children, Episodic buffer, Nonverbal intelligence, Phonological loop, Visuospatial sketchpad, Working memory",
author = "S. Gray and S. Green and Mary Alt and T. Hogan and T. Kuo and S. Brinkley and N. Cowan",
year = "2017",
month = "2",
day = "1",
doi = "10.1016/j.jml.2016.06.004",
language = "English (US)",
volume = "92",
pages = "183--201",
journal = "Journal of Memory and Language",
issn = "0749-596X",
publisher = "Academic Press Inc.",

}

TY - JOUR

T1 - The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence

AU - Gray, S.

AU - Green, S.

AU - Alt, Mary

AU - Hogan, T.

AU - Kuo, T.

AU - Brinkley, S.

AU - Cowan, N.

PY - 2017/2/1

Y1 - 2017/2/1

N2 - This study investigated the structure of working memory in young school-age children by testing the fit of three competing theoretical models using a wide variety of tasks. The best fitting models were then used to assess the relationship between working memory and nonverbal measures of fluid reasoning (Gf) and visual processing (Gv) intelligence. One hundred sixty-eight English-speaking 7–9 year olds with typical development, from three states, participated. Results showed that Cowan's three-factor embedded processes model fit the data slightly better than Baddeley and Hitch's (1974) three-factor model (specified according to Baddeley, 1986) and decisively better than Baddeley's (2000) four-factor model that included an episodic buffer. The focus of attention factor in Cowan's model was a significant predictor of Gf and Gv. The results suggest that the focus of attention, rather than storage, drives the relationship between working memory, Gf, and Gv in young school-age children. Our results do not rule out the Baddeley and Hitch model, but they place constraints on both it and Cowan's model. A common attentional component is needed for feature binding, running digit span, and visual short-term memory tasks; phonological storage is separate, as is a component of central executive processing involved in task manipulation. The results contribute to a zeitgeist in which working memory models are coming together on common ground (cf. Cowan, Saults, & Blume, 2014; Hu, Allen, Baddeley, & Hitch, 2016).

AB - This study investigated the structure of working memory in young school-age children by testing the fit of three competing theoretical models using a wide variety of tasks. The best fitting models were then used to assess the relationship between working memory and nonverbal measures of fluid reasoning (Gf) and visual processing (Gv) intelligence. One hundred sixty-eight English-speaking 7–9 year olds with typical development, from three states, participated. Results showed that Cowan's three-factor embedded processes model fit the data slightly better than Baddeley and Hitch's (1974) three-factor model (specified according to Baddeley, 1986) and decisively better than Baddeley's (2000) four-factor model that included an episodic buffer. The focus of attention factor in Cowan's model was a significant predictor of Gf and Gv. The results suggest that the focus of attention, rather than storage, drives the relationship between working memory, Gf, and Gv in young school-age children. Our results do not rule out the Baddeley and Hitch model, but they place constraints on both it and Cowan's model. A common attentional component is needed for feature binding, running digit span, and visual short-term memory tasks; phonological storage is separate, as is a component of central executive processing involved in task manipulation. The results contribute to a zeitgeist in which working memory models are coming together on common ground (cf. Cowan, Saults, & Blume, 2014; Hu, Allen, Baddeley, & Hitch, 2016).

KW - Children

KW - Episodic buffer

KW - Nonverbal intelligence

KW - Phonological loop

KW - Visuospatial sketchpad

KW - Working memory

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=84978086113&partnerID=8YFLogxK

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=84978086113&partnerID=8YFLogxK

U2 - 10.1016/j.jml.2016.06.004

DO - 10.1016/j.jml.2016.06.004

M3 - Article

VL - 92

SP - 183

EP - 201

JO - Journal of Memory and Language

JF - Journal of Memory and Language

SN - 0749-596X

ER -